Competition between human females has likely contributed to hidden ovulation

Competition for partners between prehistoric human women may have contributed to ‘hidden ovulation’ – a lack of remarkable physical evidence that a woman is fertile, experts say.

Using computer models, American researchers found evidence that hidden ovulation in humans – which is uncommon in the animal kingdom – has evolved to allow females to hide their fertility status from other females.

This would have helped avoid conflict between women, perhaps driven by aggression towards potential male partner rivals.

Previously, scientists thought women evolved to hide ovulation from men to encourage them to help care for children.

The new research shows that the origin of a hidden ovulation may be much more focused on women than previously thought.

Human females have evolved to hide physical signs of when they are ovulating - meaning males are no wiser

Human females have evolved to hide physical signs of when they are ovulating – meaning males are no wiser

“The study of human evolution tends to look at things from a male perspective,” said senior study author Athena Aktipis, associate professor of psychology at Arizona State University in the USA.

Even adaptations specific to women – such as their social behaviors and hidden ovulation – have been viewed in terms of how men shape them.

“Our computational model shows that female sociality is about much more than securing male investments.”

Human women are believed to have hidden ovulation, because there is no outward physiological sign, neither for a woman herself, nor for others, that ovulation is taking place.

As a result, women rely on useful tools such as charts, test strips, apps or wearable technology to identify periods of fertility.

In contrast, some animals, such as baboons, undergo marked physical changes during an ovulation period, particularly swelling of the perineal skin.

Gradually during human evolution, it is likely that female fertility became increasingly difficult to detect from an observer’s point of view.

For nearly half a century, the evolution of hidden ovulation in human women has been explained by a theory called the male investment hypothesis.

Human women rely on tools such as charts, test strip apps, smart monitors, and wearable technology to identify periods of fertility

Human women rely on tools such as charts, test strip apps, smart monitors, and wearable technology to identify periods of fertility

Essentially, the theory suggests that concealed ovulation was helpful in helping male partners raise and support children.

This hypothesis has been the predominant explanation for hidden ovulation for decades, although it has undergone little empirical testing and has not been formally modeled to date.

But female primates don’t just interact with males – they communicate with each other, sometimes collaborate, and sometimes they are in conflict.

“I puzzled over the male investment hypothesis for years, and since you can’t argue with a verbal hypothesis, I started testing it,” Aktipis said.

At the same time, Aktipis was working on ‘female sociality’ – a term to describe female individuals in an animal population who tend to associate in groups.

“I noticed that females could be aggressive towards other females showing ovulatory signals, which would then provide an advantage in hiding ovulation.”

Sexual swelling in a baboon.  In general, the skin around a female baboon's perineum shows cyclic changes in size, color, and firmness throughout a menstrual cycle

Sexual swelling in a baboon. In general, the skin around a female baboon’s perineum shows cyclic changes in size, color, and firmness throughout a menstrual cycle

This theory, called “the female rivalry hypothesis,” is now an alternative and compelling argument about how hidden ovulation evolved.

Ovulatory signals would make women stand out more as potential love competitors for a male partner.

Evolutionary adaptations in humans occur on the timescale of many generations, making it difficult to test whether and how traits can evolve.

Aktipis and colleagues therefore tested the hypothesis of female rivalry using computer modeling, which allows researchers to test ideas that are difficult to test in the real world.

In agent-based computer models, an ‘agent’ represents a person whose behavior can be programmed and analyzed.

Each agent follows a specific set of rules and can communicate with other agents and the environment.

In the model developed to test the female rivalry hypothesis, male and female agents followed rules that govern their movement, reproductive behavior, and attractiveness.

The male agents varied in terms of their promiscuity – promiscuous men did not work with women to raise subsequent children, while male agents who were not promiscuous hung out to share resources and support future children.

Female officers had physical cues that indicate when they were ovulating or if ovulation was hidden.

The female agents can also behave aggressively towards each other.

The female and male agents interacted with each other and had the opportunity to procreate and form parenting partnerships.

The model supported the female rivalry hypothesis by showing that women who concealed ovulation fared better, the team found.

They had more children, avoided female-female aggression, and managed to establish parenting relationships with men.

“Work in the social sciences tends to assume that male cognition and behavior is the standard,” said study first author Jaimie Arona Krems, assistant professor of psychology at Oklahoma State University.

But women regularly face some unique challenges, especially in their interactions with other women.

This work is partly the result of taking that idea seriously.

“If we do that, I think we will learn more, not just about the feminine mind, but about the human mind as well.”

The research team also used the model to test the male investment hypothesis, by running scenarios that did not allow women to behave aggressively towards each other.

But there was no apparent benefit of hiding ovulation in this scenario, again suggesting that hidden ovulation evolved due to interactions with other females.

“This work represents a necessary shift in thinking about how human females have evolved,” Aktipis said.

“Female sociality and other adaptations are not just about securing male investments, even if that has long been the underlying assumption about the purpose of female social behavior.”

The study is published in Nature Human Behavior.

How women can tell when other females are ovulating from cues on their face – and then try to hide their partners from the ‘threat’ of these fertile ladies

Experiments in 2016 showed that females can also see ovulating females simply by looking at their face.

And it was thought that this ‘skill’ could help women hold on to their partners.

Recent research has shown that men find images of ovulating women more attractive than the same people taken in the later, less fertile phase of their menstrual cycle.

In fact, females have been reported to dance, walk, sound, smell and look more attractive during their most fertile days.

Men’s ability to recognize signs of fertility matters to them as it increases their chances of passing on their genes.

While some experts believe there is no benefit for women to pick up on each other’s ovulatory signals, others say it is helpful when it comes to competing for men.

Researchers from the University of Bern, Switzerland, have set up experiments to investigate how fertile and non-fertile women interact.

They hypothesized that ovulating women could be considered a reproductive threat and luring men away from their partners.

In an online study, 160 women were shown photo pairs where one image was generated when a woman was most fertile and another in the less fertile phase of the cycle.

Participants were asked to indicate which face they found more attractive.

Another experiment in a lab performed the same test with 60 women.

In addition to choosing the more attractive face, these participants were asked which woman would be most likely to steal their own date.

Blood tests were done to record the women’s hormone levels.

Both experiments showed that, unlike men, women found neither ‘fertile’ or ‘less fertile’ faces more attractive than each other.

This came as a surprise to the researchers who expected women to find the faces of their fertile peers more attractive.

But ‘naturally cycling’ women with higher estradiol levels who were not on the pill were more likely to choose the fertile face as one of a woman who would lure her date away.

These results imply a role of estradiol [a type of oestrogen produced in the ovaries] in evaluating other women competing for reproduction, ‘says the study, published in the Royal Society’s Journal, Biology Letters.

The finding is consistent with studies finding positive associations between estradiol levels and competitive behavior in women, such as greater emotional response to sexual infidelity.

“Our data suggests that estrogen in women is more related to intra-sexual competition than to testosterone,” the scientists write.

“Ovulatory women (who are currently fertile) are a greater threat to women with high estradiol levels (who are currently not fertile but have high potential fertility).”

.Source